Are we able to differentiate between the vocal emissions of certain primates? A team from the University of Geneva (UNIGE) asked volunteers to categorise the vocalisations of three species of great apes (Hominidae) and humans. During each exposure to these ''onomatopoeia'', brain activity was measured. Unlike previous studies, the scientists reveal that phylogenetic proximity -- or kinship -- is not the only factor influencing our ability to identify these sounds. Acoustic proximity -- the type of frequencies emitted -- is also a determining factor. These results show how the human brain has evolved to process the vocal emissions of some of our closest cousins more efficiently. Find out more in the journal Cerebral Cortex Communications.
Our ability to process verbal language is not based solely on semantics, i.e. the meaning and combination of linguistic units.
Other parameters come into play, such as prosody, which includes pauses, accentuation and intonation.
Affective bursts -- ''Aaaah!'' or ''Oh!'' for example -- are also part of this, and we share these with our primate cousins.
They contribute to the meaning and understanding of our vocal communications.
When such a vocal message is emitted, these sounds are processed by the frontal and orbitofrontal regions of our brain.
The function of these two areas is, among other things, to integrate sensory and contextual information leading to a decision.
Are they activated in the same way when we are exposed to the emotional vocalisations of our close cousins the chimpanzees, macaques and bonobos?
Are we able to differentiate between them?
MRI scans with headphones on
A UNIGE team sought to find out by exposing a group of 25 volunteers to various human and simian vocalisations.
''The participants were placed in an MRI scanner and were given headphones.
After a short period of familiarisation with the different types of vocalisations, each participant had to categorise them, i.e. identify to which species they belonged,'' explains Leonardo Ceravolo, senior lecturer at the UNIGE's Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, and first author of the study.
These vocalisations were of the affiliative type, i.e. linked to a positive interaction, or of the agonistic type, i.e. linked to a threat or distress.
The human vocalisations came from databases recorded by actors.
The simian ones came from field recordings made as part of previous research.
This study is the first of its kind to include bonobo vocalisations.
Bonobos, not so close cousins
The results show that for macaque and chimpanzee vocalisations, the frontal and orbitofrontal regions of the participants were activated in a similar way to human vocalisations.
The participants were able to differentiate between them easily.
On the other hand, when confronted with the ''sounds'' of bonobos, also close cousins of humans, the involved cerebral areas were much less activated, and categorisation was at chance level.
''It was thought that kinship between species -- the 'phylogenetic distance' -- was the main parameter for having the ability, or not, to recognise these different vocalisations.
We thought that the closer we were genetically, the more important this ability was,'' explains Didier Grandjean, full professor at the Swiss Center for Affective Sciences and at the UNIGE's Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, who led the study.
''Our results show that a second parameter comes into play: acoustic distance.
The further the dynamics of the acoustic parameters, such as the frequencies used, are from those of humans, the less certain frontal regions are activated.
Read more at Science Daily
No comments:
Post a Comment